Journal of Animal Science and Technology
Korean Society of Animal Sciences and Technology
RESEARCH ARTICLE

Association between the plasma concentration of melatonin and behavioral temperament in horses

Yubin Song1https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6336-0500, Junyoung Kim1https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0711-5479, Youngjae Park2https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0038-7615, Minjung Yoon1,3,4,*https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9112-1796
1Department of Animal Science and Biotechnology, Kyungpook National University, Sangju 37224, Korea
2Department of Equine Industry and Sports with Therapeutic Riding, Jeonju Kijeon College, Jeonju 54989, Korea
3Department of Horse, Companion and Wild Animal Science, Kyungpook National University, Sangju 37224, Korea
4Research Center for Horse Industry, Kyungpook National University, Sangju 37224, Korea
*Corresponding author: Minjung Yoon, Department of Animal Science and Biotechnology, Kyungpook National University, Sangju 37224, Korea. Tel: +82-54-530-1233, E-mail: mjyoon@knu.ac.kr

© Copyright 2023 Korean Society of Animal Science and Technology. This is an Open-Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Received: Dec 12, 2022; Revised: Jan 17, 2023; Accepted: Jan 22, 2023

Published Online: Sep 30, 2023

Abstract

Aggression in horses may cause serious accidents during riding and non-riding activities. Hence, predicting the temperament of horses is essential for selecting suitable horses and ensuring safety during the activity. In certain animals, such as hamsters, plasma melatonin concentrations have been correlated with aggressive behavior. However, whether this relationship applies to horses remains unclear. To address this research gap, this study aimed to evaluate differences in the plasma melatonin concentrations among horses of different breeds, ages, and sexes and examine the correlation between plasma melatonin concentrations and the temperament of the horses, including docility, affinity, dominance, and trainability. Blood samples from 32 horses were collected from the Horse Industry Complex Center of Jeonju Kijeon College. The docility, affinity, dominance, and trainability of the horses were assessed by three professional trainers who were well-acquainted with the horses. Plasma melatonin concentrations were measured using an enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay. The consequent values were compared between the horses of different breeds, ages, and sexes using a three-way analysis of variance and least significant difference post hoc test. Linear regression analysis was employed to identify the relationship between plasma melatonin concentrations and docility, affinity, dominance, and trainability. The results showed that the plasma melatonin concentrations significantly differed with breeds in Thoroughbred and cold-blooded horses. However, there were no differences in the plasma melatonin concentrations between the horse ages and sexes. Furthermore, plasma melatonin concentrations did not exhibit a significant correlation with the ranking of docility, affinity, dominance, and trainability.

Keywords: Melatonin; Behavioral temperament; Horse

INTRODUCTION

Temperament refers to individual differences in biological behavioral characteristics based on the behavior and exhibits various behavioral characteristics [1]. The temperament of animals is one of the key factors affecting the degree of interrelationship with humans [2]. Thus, predicting the temperament of animals may prevent accidents during activities involving large animals, such as horses [3]. Visser and coworkers [4] indicated that knowing a horse’s temperaments is essential for safe riding or non-riding activities. Furthermore, knowing the temperament of horses improves their welfare by ensuring appropriate management. Kilgour [5] noted that animal’s temperament could be determed based on the evaluation of animal behavior. However, because such behavior evaluation can often be subjective, objective, and practical assessment methods for the temperament of horses are warranted.

Neurotransmitters, the chemical messengers between neurons and target receptors mediate all neuronal phenomena [6]. Neurotransmitters influence various social behavior in animals, including affinity and depression [7]. For example, to alleviate the symptoms of behavioral disorders, such as those of depression and anxiety, serotonin is released into the synapse [810]. Oxytocin is a neuropeptide that alleviates the symptoms of depression, anxiety, and repetitive behavior in mice [11] [12]. In horses, serotonin and oxytocin act as a factor to determine equine docility and friendliness to humans [13].

Melatonin is a hormone secreted by the pineal glands in the diencephalon, produced by sunlight exposure during the day and released at night. Kilic and coworkers [14] reported that melatonin secretion was associated with various social behaviors, including hyperactivity, anxiety, and depression. One study relating melatonin concentrations to behavior reported that animals exhibiting hyperactivity demonstrated a shorter duration of melatonin signaling and frequent nocturnal melatonin peaks compared with the control group [15]. Remarkably, melatonin has been related to aggressive behavior in animals. In male Syrian hamsters, animals exhibiting higher melatonin concentrations demonstrated more aggressive behavior [16]. In rats, melatonin exhibited neuroprotective action and improved insomnia-induced anxiety [17,18]. Furthermore, in a study involving humans, the participants who were administered melatonin exhibited more aggression in specific situations than participants in the control group who were administered a placebo [19]. Hence, these results indicate the association of melatonin with aggressive behavior in nonhuman mammals as well as humans. However, the relationship between melatonin and the temperaments of horses is yet to be elucidated.

Neurotransmitter secretions differ depending on the breed, age, and sex of animals. For example, the behavior of domestic dogs differs depending on their breed [20]. Sex in rats and age in monkeys have been associated with differences in their neurotransmitter systems [21,22]. Thus, differences in melatonin concentration according to breed, age, and sex in horses are worth examining.

This study aimed to evaluate differences in the plasma concentration of melatonin among the different breeds, ages, and sexes of horses and to examine the correlation between plasma concentrations and the temperament of horses, including docility, affinity, dominance, and trainability.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

This study was conducted at the Horse Industry Complex Center of Jeonju Kijeon College in Korea. The Animal Experiment Ethics Committee of Kyungpook National University approved the protocol for animal use (2022-0483). In total, 32 horses were included in this study, including 15 Thoroughbreds, 2 Ponies, 7 Warmbloods, 1 Halla horse, 6 cold-blooded horses (Connemara pony and Halflinger), and 1 quarterhorse. The sexes of the horse used in the experiment consisted of 12 geldings, 19 mares, and 1 stallion. The ages of horses ranged from 5 to 26 years (9.0 ± 1.01 years). The ages of horses ranged from five to twenty-five years. The horses were housed in 3.5 m × 3.5 m stalls with an automatic water supply and fed timothy hay (1.5% of body weight) and commercial concentrates (0.5% of body weight) per day.

Blood sampling

Approximately 10 mL of blood was collected from the jugular vein of the horses. The blood samples were collected from 10 to 11 PM and loaded in EDTA tubes (BD Vacutainer, Franklin Lakes, NJ, USA) and maintained in a 4°C icebox during transportation. To separate the plasma from blood samples, a centrifuge machine was used with 1,500×g for 110 min at 25°C. The plasma was stored at −70°C refrigerator before analysis.

Enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay analysis

To determine the plasma melatonin concentrations, a horse melatonin ELISA kit (HREB0045, Assay Genie, Windsor, Dublin, Ireland) with a sensitivity of 10 pg/mL was employed. Sunrise Absorbance Microplate Reader (Tecan, Männedorf, Switzerland) was used to evaluate the plasma samples at 450 nm. The intra-assay and interassay coefficients of variability were 5.9% and 9.1%, respectively.

Assessment of horse temperaments

Docility, affinity, dominance, and trainability were scored by three professional trainers who had been training the horses for at least five years and were well-acquainted with the horses. The temperament of each horse was assessed based on the criteria shown in Table 1. Docility was scored based on the behavioral response of the horses in an unfamiliar situation and the time required to catch the horse. Affinity was evaluated according to the distance the horses maintained from humans and their response to humans. Trainability was determined as per the willingness of the horses to participate in training and the time required to achieve the training goal. The dominance of a horse was assessed by it gaining the upper hand among other horses in social settings or when competing over food. The temperament of each horse was determined by averaging the scores given by the three professional trainers. The scores for each aspect of the behavioral temperament of the horses ranged from 0 to 5 points. The scores were categorized into three grades: low (0–1 point), medium (2–3 points), and high (4–5 points).

Table 1. Assessment of horse temperament and scoring system
Temperament Behavioral characteristics Scoring
Docility Behavioral response in an unfamiliar situation
The time it takes to catch
High
(4–5 score)
Affinity The degree of distance between humans
The time it takes to get familiar with strangers
Medium
(2–3 score)
Dominance The act of gaining the upper hand with other horses or over food Low
(0–1 score)
Trainability The willingness to participate in training
The time it takes to achieve the goal of training
Download Excel Table
Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS V25 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA). A three-way analysis of variance with the least significant difference post hoc test was used to compare plasma melatonin concentrations among the horses of different breeds, ages, and sex. Linear regression was employed to characterize the relationship between plasma melatonin concentrations and docility, affinity, dominance, and trainability rankings. To increase normality and accuracy, all raw data were converted to log values. Raw data exceeding the first and third quartiles were considered outliers and removed.

RESULT

Melatonin concentration among breeds, ages, and sexes in horses

Differences in breeds, ages, and sexes according to plasma concentration of melatonin were analyzed. The melatonin concentrations between cold-blooded horses and Thoroughbreds were significantly different (Table 2). The mean concentration of melatonin in cold-blooded horses was significantly lower than in Thoroughbreds. Conversely, the mean melatonin concentrations in Warmbloods did not significantly differ from that of other breeds. Furthermore, plasma melatonin concentrations did not vary with age (Table 3) or between geldings and mares (Table 4). The mean melatonin concentration demonstrated a significant interaction with the horse breeds (p < 0.05); however, melatonin concentrations did not differ significantly with the age and sex of the horses.

Table 2. Plasma concentration of melatonin among different horse breeds1)
Neurotransmitter Breed of horses
Thoroughbred
(n = 15)
Warmblood
(n = 7)
Cold-blooded horse
(n = 6)
MEL (pg/mL) 120.3 ± 28.66a 134.1 ± 59.03ab 34.8 ± 5.07b

Data represent the mean ± SEM.

Means with a different superscript in the same row are significantly different (p < 0.05).

Download Excel Table
Table 3. Plasma concentration of melatonin among different horse ages1)
Neurotransmitter Age of horses
1 to 5 years
(n = 5)
6 to 12 years
(n = 11)
More than 13 years
(n = 7)
MEL (pg/mL) 156.6 ± 70.03 127.1 ± 38.32 126.1 ± 47.67

Data represent the mean ± SEM.

Download Excel Table
Table 4. Plasma concentration of melatonin among different horse sexes1)
Neurotransmitter Sex of horses
Gelding (n = 12) Mare (n = 11)
MEL (pg/mL) 123.7 ± 35.72 143.5 ± 43.29

Data represent the mean ± SEM.

Download Excel Table
Differences in the plasma concentration of melatonin according to horse use

The differences in the plasma concentration of melatonin according to the uses of horses were confirmed. Given that cold-blooded horses exhibited significantly lower plasma melatonin concentrations than the other horse breeds, their data were excluded from the horse use analysis. Horses in the training program (who were being trained) exhibited the highest mean melatonin concentration; however, the difference was not statistically significant.

Relation between melatonin concentrations with docility

The graph of linear regression showed the relation between the plasma concentration of melatonin and the ranking of docility. Melatonin concentration showed a coefficient (R2) of 0.01 (p = 0.90, Fig. 1). There was no correlation between the plasma concentration of melatonin and the ranking of the docility of horses.

jast-65-5-1094-g1
Fig. 1. Linear regression was used to confirm the correlation between the plasma concentration of melatonin and the docility of horses. The linear correlation was not significant between melatonin concentration and docility.
Download Original Figure
Relation between melatonin concentrations with an affinity

The graph of linear regression showed the relation between the plasma concentration of melatonin and the ranking of affinity. The linear regression of plasma melatonin concentrations and affinity had a coefficient (R2) of 0.01 (p = 0.90, Fig. 2), indicating no significant relationship.

jast-65-5-1094-g2
Fig. 2. Linear regression was used to confirm the correlation between the plasma concentration of melatonin and the affinity of horses. The linear correlation was not significant between melatonin concentration and affinity.
Download Original Figure
Relation between melatonin concentrations with trainability

The graph of linear regression showed the relation between melatonin concentration and trainability. The linear regression of plasma melatonin concentrations and trainability had a coefficient (R2) of 0.01 (p = 0.90, Fig. 3), indicating no significant relationship.

jast-65-5-1094-g3
Fig. 3. Linear regression was used to confirm the correlation between the plasma concentration of melatonin and the dominance of horses. The linear correlation was not significant between melatonin concentration and dominance.
Download Original Figure
Relation between melatonin concentrations with dominance

The graph of linear regression showed the relation between the plasma concentration of melatonin and the ranking of dominance. The linear regression of plasma melatonin concentrations and dominance had a coefficient (R2) of 0.08 (p = 0.623, Fig. 4), indicating no correlation.

jast-65-5-1094-g4
Fig. 4. Linear regression was used to confirm the correlation between melatonin and horse trainability. The linear correlation was not significant between melatonin and trainability.
Download Original Figure

DISCUSSION

The variation of melatonin concentration in the different breeds, ages, and sexes

In this study, the variations of melatonin concentration in the different breeds, ages, and sexes were evaluated. Plasma melatonin concentrations were significantly lower in cold-blooded horses than in Thoroughbreds, suggesting varying melatonin concentrations between these breeds (Table 2). Horses are known to differ in personality according to their breeds [23]. For example, Thoroughbreds were specifically bred for horse racing [24], whereas cold-blooded horses have been employed as workhorses for a long time, adapted from natural conditions [25]. Furthermore, the personality of horses may differ with the degree of their domestication and training status [26]. Therefore, horse breeds may exhibit different average melatonin concentrations owing to different breeds and training. Thoroughbreds are considered hot-blooded horses and thus may differ from cold-blooded horses; hot-blooded horses exhibit an active and sensitive personality, while cold-blooded horses exhibit a mild personality [27]. These observations suggest that horses exhibiting higher melatonin concentrations demonstrate a more active personality. However, Thoroughbreds were the only hot-blooded horses included in this study. To further confirm differences in plasma melatonin concentrations between hot-blooded and cold-blooded horses, other hot-blooded horse breeds should be analyzed.

The plasma melatonin concentrations of Warmbloods also tended to be higher than that of cold-blooded horses, although the difference was not statistically significant. Warmblood horses exhibit large individual variations in plasma melatonin concentrations, probably owing to different training methods, i.e., some Warmblood horses were trained for dressage and others for show jumping. This could be a reason for the variation in the plasma concentration of melatonin within the Warmblood horses. In this study, these two groups were combined as Warmblood horses owing to the small population. Thus, future studies should compare plasma melatonin concentrations between the two groups of Warmblood horses.

Variations in plasma melatonin concentrations according to age and sex were also evaluated. Cold-blooded horses were excluded from this analysis because of the association of this breed with plasma melatonin concentrations. There was no difference in plasma melatonin concentrations according to age (approximately 1–5 years, 6–12 years, > 13 years; Table 3), suggesting that melatonin concentrations were unrelated to age. This result was in accordance with that of a previous study reporting that melatonin concentrations in humans were unrelated to age, showing similar melatonin concentrations in healthy older (aged 65–81 years) and young (aged 18–30 years) individuals [28]. These results support our finding that the plasma concentration of melatonin in horses is not associated with age. In this study, there was no difference in melatonin concentration between geldings and mares. Furthermore, no difference in melatonin concentrations was detected between geldings and mares, indicating that melatonin concentrations in horses were sex hormone-independent. This result is consistent with that of a previous study reporting no difference in melatonin secretion rates between men and women [29].

In the present study, a correlation was observed between plasma melatonin concentrations and horse use (Table 5). The horses included in this study were chiefly used for horse competitions and riding lessons, whereas some were being trained. As most of the young horses were cold-blooded, they were excluded from the analysis of horse uses. Horses in the training program tended to exhibit higher melatonin concentrations than the other horses. Notably, the horses used in competitions and for riding lessons were extensively trained for safe riding contrary to the horses in the training program. Compared with well-trained horses, partly trained horses tended to exhibit more aggressive behavior owing to their inexperience with humans. This finding is consistent with that of a previous study showing that melatonin concentrations affected aggressive behavior in rodent and fish models [30].

Table 5. Plasma concentration of melatonin among different horse uses1)
Neurotransmitter Use of horses
Competition
(n = 8)
In the training program
(n = 7)
Riding lesson
(n = 7)
MEL (pg/mL) 71.7 ± 32.54 126.0 ± 33.13 81.7 ± 14.20

Data represent the mean ± SEM.

Download Excel Table
The relation between melatonin and temperaments of horses

Docility is defined as the gentleness of personality or mind. To ensure safe riding and interactions, using horses exhibiting high docility is essential. Horses demonstrating low docility may engender hyperactivity and related problems, such as impulsiveness, inability to concentrate, and being easily distracted. In this study, plasma melatonin concentrations were not correlated with docility ranking, suggesting that melatonin is not associated with this aspect of temperament in horses. This result is in accordance with those reported previously. In humans, hyperactivity and plasma melatonin concentrations were not correlated [31]. In addition, melatonin treatment did not reduce the symptoms of patients with hyperactivity [32]. These results suggest that melatonin is not involved in controlling docility in horses. Similarly, there was no correlation between plasma melatonin concentrations and affinity in horses. Among horses, affinity is assessed based on friendliness to other horses and humans. Horses with high affinity tend to have good relationships with their herd [33]. Furthermore, affinity has been linked to social skills. Both animals and humans with low affinity may lack social skills, causing behavioral disorders, including depression and anxiety [34].

Diminished social skills can also produce various pathological changes in the brain, thereby leading to behavioral changes [35]. Depression, caused by the lack of monoamines, is frequently treated with serotonin, a melatonin precursor. Serotonin increases the levels of neuromodulators and growth factors by activating cell signaling pathways, eventually restoring monoamine synapses [36,37]. However, Waterman and coworkers [38] reported that the levels of 6-hydroxy melatonin sulfate, a melatonin metabolite, did not differ between people with depressive disorders and healthy controls, suggesting that melatonin is not related to depressive behaviors. Thus, the findings of these previous studies support our finding that melatonin is not associated with affinity in horses.

Dominant animals typically take the lead position during sexual and feeding activities. Previous studies have reported melatonin increases the propensity to aggression in animals and humans. For example, in female Siberian hamsters, melatonin induces aggressive behavior by regulating adrenal androgens [39]. In humans, high melatonin concentrations encourage aggressive behaviors, such as irritability and anger [19]. Exogenous melatonin administration has also been demonstrated to increase aggression in animals and humans. However, we found no relationship between plasma melatonin concentrations and dominant and aggressive behaviors in horses. Notably, in this study, the relationship between endogenous melatonin instead of exogenous melatonin with aggression was analyzed. Based on the results of these studies, we hypothesized that short-term aggressive behavior can be elicited in response to exogenous melatonin administration. However, endogenous melatonin concentrations cannot be used as an indicator of the degree of aggression.

Trainability refers to the ability to learn and quickly accept training. Trainability varies with individual abilities, such as learning and memory. Several lines of evidence suggest a relationship between melatonin and learning. For example, melatonin has been shown to improve symptoms of neurodegenerative disorders, reducing neurooxidative stress as well as learning and memory deficits [4042]. Melatonin also facilitates short-term memory [43]. However, the result of this study showed no correlation between the plasma concentration and the ranking of trainability of horses. Notably, Martini and coworkers [44] found that melatonin promotes the disappearance of learned responses and demonstrates no effect on memory acquisition. Different results of these two studies could be due to the variation in observation time, interval, or stress levels. Thus, it is not clear that melatonin is associated with the training ability of other species. However, the present study suggests that the plasma concentration of melatonin cannot be used as a marker to evaluate the trainability of horses.

In conclusion, the average plasma melatonin concentrations differed according to the horse breeds. However, melatonin was not associated with docility, affinity, dominance, or trainability of the horses and is thus unlikely to be a useful biomarker for horse temperaments.

Competing interests

No potential conflict of interest relevant to this article was reported.

Funding sources

Not applicable.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to Yeonju Choi, Seongmin Kim, Youngwook Jung, and Shakeel Hafiz Muhammad (Kyungpook National University, Korea).

Availability of data and material

Upon reasonable request, the datasets of this study can be available from the corresponding author.

Authors’ contributions

Conceptualization: Song Y, Yoon M.

Data curation: Song Y, Yoon M.

Formal analysis: Song Y, Kim J.

Methodology: Song Y, Kim J, Park Y.

Software: Song Y.

Validation: Song Y, Yoon M.

Investigation: Song Y, Park Y.

Writing - original draft: Song Y.

Writing - review & editing: Song Y, Kim J, Park Y, Yoon M.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study protocols were reviewed and approved by the Animal Experimentation Ethics Committee of Kyungpook National University (2022-0483).

REFERENCES

1.

Larsen RJ, Diener E. Affect intensity as an individual difference characteristic: a review. J Res Pers. 1987; 21:1-39

2.

Martin JGA, Réale D. Animal temperament and human disturbance: implications for the response of wildlife to tourism. Behav Process. 2008; 77:66-72

3.

Grandin T. Safe handling of large animals. Philadelphia, PA: Occupational Medicine. 1999

4.

Visser EK, van Reenen CG, Hopster H, Schilder MBH, Knaap JH, Barneveld A, et al. Quantifying aspects of young horses’ temperament: consistency of behavioural variables. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2001; 74:241-58

5.

Kilgour R. The open-field test as an assessment of the temperament of dairy cows. Anim Behav. 1975; 23:615-24

6.

Tyler WJ, Perrett SP, Pozzo-Miller LD. The role of neurotrophins in neurotransmitter release. Neuroscientist. 2002; 8:524-31

7.

Levi G, Raiteri M. Carrier-mediated release of neurotransmitters. Trends Neurosci. 1993; 16:415-9

8.

Jonnakuty C, Gragnoli C. What do we know about serotonin?. J Cell Physiol. 2008; 217:301-6

9.

Bellivier F, Leboyer M, Courtet P, Buresi C, Beaufils B, Samolyk D, et al. Association between the tryptophan hydroxylase gene and manic-depressive illness. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998; 55:33-7

10.

Lucki I. The spectrum of behaviors influenced by serotonin. Biol Psychiatry. 1998; 44:151-62

11.

Swanson LW, Kuypers HGJM. The paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus: cytoarchitectonic subdivisions and organization of projections to the pituitary, dorsal vagal complex, and spinal cord as demonstrated by retrograde fluorescence double-labeling methods. J Comp Neurol. 1980; 194:555-70

12.

Wang Y, Zhao S, Liu X, Zheng Y, Li L, Meng S. Oxytocin improves animal behaviors and ameliorates oxidative stress and inflammation in autistic mice. Biomed Pharmacother. 2018; 107:262-9

13.

Kim J, Yoon M. The effect of serotonin and oxytocin on equine docility and friendliness to humans. J Vet Behav. 2022; 50:18-22

14.

Kilic E, Kilic Ü, Bacigaluppi M, Guo Z, Abdallah NB, Wolfer DP, et al. Delayed melatonin administration promotes neuronal survival, neurogenesis and motor recovery, and attenuates hyperactivity and anxiety after mild focal cerebral ischemia in mice. J Pineal Res. 2008; 45:142-8

15.

Nováková M, Paclt I, Ptáček R, Kuželová H, Hájek I, Sumová A. Salivary melatonin rhythm as a marker of the circadian system in healthy children and those with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Chronobiol Int. 2011; 28:630-7

16.

Jasnow AM, Huhman KL, Bartness TJ, Demas GE. Short days and exogenous melatonin increase aggression of male Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). Horm Behav. 2002; 42:13-20

17.

Wang X, Wang Z, Cao J, Dong Y, Chen Y. Melatonin ameliorates anxiety-like behaviors induced by sleep deprivation in mice: role of oxidative stress, neuroinflammation, autophagy and apoptosis. Brain Res Bull. 2021; 174:161-72

18.

Nava F, Carta G. Melatonin reduces anxiety induced by lipopolysaccharide in the rat. Neurosci Lett. 2001; 307:57-60

19.

Liu J, Zhong R, Xiong W, Liu H, Eisenegger C, Zhou X. Melatonin increases reactive aggression in humans. Psychopharmacology. 2017; 234:2971-8

20.

Mehrkam LR, Wynne CDL. Behavioral differences among breeds of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris): current status of the science. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2014; 155:12-27

21.

De Vries GJ. Sex differences in neurotransmitter systems. J Neuroendocrinol. 1990; 2:1-13

22.

Wenk GL, Pierce DJ, Struble RG, Price DL, Cork LC. Age-related changes in multiple neurotransmitter systems in the monkey brain. Neurobiol Aging. 1989; 10:11-9

23.

Lloyd AS, Martin JE, Bornett-Gauci HLI, Wilkinson RG. Horse personality: variation between breeds. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008; 112:369-83

24.

Bower MA, McGivney BA, Campana MG, Gu J, Andersson LS, Barrett E, et al. The genetic origin and history of speed in the Thoroughbred racehorse. Nat Commun. 2012; 3:643

25.

Goodwin D. Horse behaviour: evolution, domestication and feralisation.In In: Waran N, editor.editor The welfare of horses. Dordrecht: Springer. 2007; p p. 1-18

26.

Widi TSM, Rakasiwi GA, Nugroho T, Widyas N. Personality assessment of different horse breeds trained for military purposes. IOP Conf Ser Earth Environ Sci. 2008; 207:012020

27.

Braybrook Z. The use of a startle test to determine the differences between hot-blooded and cold-blooded equines–is the stereotype true?. J Anim Sci Technol. Forthcoming 2023

28.

Zeitzer JM, Daniels JE, Duffy JF, Klerman EB, Shanahan TL, Dijk DJ, et al. Do plasma melatonin concentrations decline with age?. Am J Med. 1999; 107:432-6

29.

Tetsuo M, Poth M, Markey SP. Melatonin metabolite excretion during childhood and puberty. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1982; 55:311-3

30.

Paribello P, Manchia M, Bosia M, Pinna F, Carpiniello B, Comai S. Melatonin and aggressive behavior: a systematic review of the literature on preclinical and clinical evidence. J Pineal Res. 2022; 72e12794

31.

Van der Heijden KB, Smits MG, Van Someren EJW, Richard Ridderinkhof K, Boudewijn Gunning W. Effect of melatonin on sleep, behavior, and cognition in ADHD and chronic sleep-onset insomnia. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007; 46:233-41

32.

Mohammadi MR, Mostafavi SA, Keshavarz SA, Eshraghian MR, Hosseinzadeh P, Hosseinzadeh-Attar MJ, et al. Melatonin effects in methylphenidate treated children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a randomized double blind clinical trial. Iran J Psychiatry. 2012; 7:87-92

33.

Briard L, Dorn C, Petit O. Personality and affinities play a key role in the organisation of collective movements in a group of domestic horses. Ethology. 2015; 121:888-902

34.

Tse WS, Bond AJ. The impact of depression on social skills: a review. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2004; 192:260-8

35.

Greenberg D, Katz A, Epstein M, Golovchiner G, Ilia R, Caspi A. Implantable cardioverter defibrillators in Israel: utilization and implantation trends. Int J Cardiol. 2002; 82:17-23

36.

Krishnan V, Nestler EJ. The molecular neurobiology of depression. Nature. 2008; 455:894-902

37.

Hagan JJ. Molecular and functional models in neuropsychiatry. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media. 2011

38.

Scott Waterman G, Ryan ND, Perel JM, Dahl RE, Birmaher B, Williamson DE, et al. Nocturnal urinary excretion of 6-hydroxymelatonin sulfate in prepubertal major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1992; 31:582-90

39.

Rendon NM, Rudolph LM, Sengelaub DR, Demas GE. The agonistic adrenal: melatonin elicits female aggression via regulation of adrenal androgens. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2015; 282:20152080

40.

Pohanka M. Alzheimer’s disease and related neurodegenerative disorders: implication and counteracting of melatonin. J Appl Biomed. 2011; 9:185-96

41.

Shen YX, Xu SY, Wei W, Sun XX, Yang J, Liu LH, et al. Melatonin reduces memory changes and neural oxidative damage in mice treated with D-galactose. J Pineal Res. 2003; 32:173-8

42.

Song TY, Lin HC, Chen CL, Wu JH, Liao JW, Hu ML. Ergothioneine and melatonin attenuate oxidative stress and protect against learning and memory deficits in C57BL/6J mice treated with D-galactose. Free Radic Res. 2014; 48:1049-60

43.

Argyriou A, Prast H, Philippu A. Melatonin facilitates short-term memory. Eur J Pharmacol. 1998; 349:159-62

44.

Martini L. Behavioral effects of pineal principles. Pineal Gland. 1971; :368-72